Stephen Krashen
The English Connection (KOTESOL). 15, 3: 1 (2011)
It
is by now well-established that input must be comprehensible to have an effect
on language acquisition and literacy development. To make sure that language
acquirers pay attention to the input, it should be interesting. But interest
may be not enough for optimal language acquisition. It may be the case that
input needs to be not just interesting but compelling.
Compelling
means that the input is so interesting you forget that it is in another
language. It means you are in a state of "flow" (Csikszentmihalyi,
1990). In flow, the concerns of everyday life and even the sense of self
disappear - our sense of time is altered and nothing but the activity itself
seems to matter. Flow occurs during reading when readers are "lost in the
book" (Nell, 1988) or in the "Reading Zone" (Atwell, 2007).
Compelling
input appears to eliminate the need for motivation, a conscious desire to
improve. When you get compelling input, you acquire whether you are interested
in improving or not.
The
evidence for the Compelling Input Hypothesis includes improvement as an
unexpected result, the many cases of those who had no conscious intention of
improving in another language or increasing their literacy, but simply got very
interested in reading. In fact, they were sometimes surprised that they had
improved.
I
included several cases like this in The Power of Reading (Krashen, 2004, pp.
22-24): Both students and teachers were surprised by the students' startling
improvement in English after they became avid readers in English.
More
recently, Lao (Lao and Krashen, 2009) described the case of Daniel, a
12-year-old boy who came to the US at age eight from China. Daniel's Mandarin
proficiency was clearly declining, despite his parents' efforts: They sent
Daniel to a Chinese heritage language school but it was clear that Daniel was
not interested in Mandarin. He was also not an enthusiastic participant in a
summer heritage language program supervised by Dr. Lao, even though it included
free reading.
Then
Dr. Lao gave Daniel a few books written in Chinese to take home. One was an
illustrated chapter book, "The Stories of A Fan Ti." Daniel loved it.
The book was a bit beyond his level, but thanks to the illustrations and his
ability to understand some of the text, Daniel was very interested in the
story, and begged his mother to read it to him. When Dr. Lao learned of this,
she loaned Daniel more books from the "A Fan Ti" series, in comic
book format. Daniel begged his mother to read more, from two to five stories
everyday. Daniel liked the books so much that he would do the dishes while his
mother read to him. Both Daniel and his mother were quite happy with this
arrangement. Daniel's Mandarin was clearly improving, but he wasn't aware of
it, nor was he particularly interested. He was only interested in the stories.
The
Compelling Input Hypothesis also explains why self-selected reading is
typically more effective than assigned reading (e.g. S.Y. Lee, 2007).
An
important conjecture is that listening to or reading compelling stories,
watching compelling movies and having conversations with truly fascinating
people is not simply another route, another option. It is possible that
compelling input is not just optimal: It may be only way we truly acquire
language.
References
Atwell,
Nancy. 2007. The Reading Zone. New York: Scholastic.
Csikszentmihalyi , M. 1990. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.
Krashen, S. 2004. The Power of Reading. Second edition. Portsmouth: Heinemann and Westport: Libraries Unlimited
Lao, C. and Krashen, S. 2008. Heritage language development: Exhortation or good stories? International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching 4 (2): 17-18.
Lee, S. Y. 2007. Revelations from Three Consecutive Studies on Extensive Reading. Regional Language Center (RELC) Journal , 38 (2), 150-170.
Nell, V. 1988. Lost in a Book. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press.
Csikszentmihalyi , M. 1990. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.
Krashen, S. 2004. The Power of Reading. Second edition. Portsmouth: Heinemann and Westport: Libraries Unlimited
Lao, C. and Krashen, S. 2008. Heritage language development: Exhortation or good stories? International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching 4 (2): 17-18.
Lee, S. Y. 2007. Revelations from Three Consecutive Studies on Extensive Reading. Regional Language Center (RELC) Journal , 38 (2), 150-170.
Nell, V. 1988. Lost in a Book. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press.