Literacy Education: Need We Start Early?
Stephen Krashen
Language and Language Teaching, Vol 3, number
2, issue 6: pp 1-7. 2014
Most people are convinced that children will
have a powerful head start in reading if they are introduced to sounds and
letters well before they start school. The conclusions of this paper are these:
For the development of English literacy, early explicit teaching fails, but
later, absorption in interesting and comprehensible reading works very well.
Early Literacy: Phonemic Awareness
Phonemic awareness (PA) is the ability to
divide a word into its component sounds, i.e.
the ability to take the word ‘pit’, and divide it into ‘pe’ ‘i’ and
‘te’. It is thus an aural ability. It is
frequently claimed that phonemic awareness is a prerequisite to learning to
read, and children must be “trained” in phonemic awareness in kindergarten and
grade 1, and even in preschool. Research and observations have however cast
doubts on this claim, and suggest that phonemic awareness, beyond the most
basic level, is the result of reading, not a cause.
No Evidence that PA Training Improves Reading Comprehension
Children who receive training in phonemic
awareness improve on tests of phonemic awareness, but there is no evidence to
suggest that PA training benefits reading comprehension, i.e. performance in
tests in which children have to understand what they read. A review of research
literature (Krashen, 2001a) produced only six studies and eleven comparisons in
which researchers attempt to see if PA training has an impact on reading
comprehension. Only three of the six studies dealt with English-speaking
children. These studies revealed low, zero and even negative scores for the
impact of PA training on comprehension, and in some studies the number of
children who underwent the training was very small. There was only one study
that reported substantial impact as well as statistically significant results
in favor of those trained in phonemic awareness. This study was conducted in
Israel with only 15 Hebrew-speaking children, who underwent PA training
(Kozminsky and Kozminsky, 1995).
Another review (Krashen and Hastings, 2011)
concluded that there is no evidence that PA training improves reading
comprehension in a second language.
Low PA reads OK
It has been widely observed that many
children with low or even no phonemic awareness learn to read quite well. Also,
many children judged to have low phonemic awareness when young, develop good
reading abilities later in life, and some adults who are excellent readers do
poorly on tests of phonemic awareness (Krashen, 2001b).
A clear example of this is provided by
Campbell and Butterworth (1985). Their subject, R.E., was a university student
who “reads at least as well as her fellow undergraduates” (p. 436). This university
student graduated from London University with second-class honors in
psychology, and performed above average on standardized tests of reading. She
had great difficulty in reading nonsense words, and while she knew the names of
all the letters, she had difficulty with the sounds corresponding to the
letters. She also performed poorly on tests of phonemic awareness and phonemic
segmentation. Campbell and Butterworth concluded:
“Since R.E.’s word reading and
spelling are good, strong claims based on the necessity of a relationship
between phonemic segmentation and manipulation skills, on the one hand, and the
development of skilled reading and writing, on the other, must be weakened” (p.
460).
For additional examples, see Krashen (2001b).
These results cast a doubt on the claim that
phonemic awareness is a prerequisite to learning to read.
PA the result of reading
Phonemic awareness beyond the initial levels
appears to be the result of reading, not the cause. This conclusion is
consistent with the observation that all but the most rudimentary aspects of
phonemic awareness emerge at about the age children learn to read (Krashen,
2003). To test this hypothesis, I conducted an informal research project. I
asked a number of people to perform the classic PA task of stripping the
initial consonant from a word such as "pit." Of course, everybody got
this right without a problem. Then I asked them to do the same with the word
"split." After some hesitation, most people got it right. I then
asked them how they did it. Universally, people reported that they spelled the
word in their mind, removed the /p/ sound, and pronounced the remainder. This
confirmed that the ability to do complex PA tasks is dependent on the ability
to read.
What all this suggests is that PA need not be
taught. It is not essential for learning
to read, and those who develop it do so from reading itself.
Phonics
Phonics is the study of the
rules relating sounds to spelling, i.e. the fact that the letter “b” is
generally pronounced as in the first sound in “bomb", but is sometimes
silent, as the last “b" in "bomb".
There are several possibilities about the
role of consciously learned phonics in reading—intensive systematic phonics,
basic phonics and zero phonics.
Intensive, Systematic Phonics
“Phonics instruction is systematic when all of
the major letter-sound correspondences are taught and covered in a clearly
defined sequence ...” (Ehri, 2004, p. 180). According to intensive systematic
phonics, we learn to read by first learning the rules ("all the major
rules") of phonics, that is, learning how letters are pronounced
(“sounding out”), and by practicing these rules while reading out loud
(“decoding to sound”). Also, our knowledge of phonics must be deliberately taught
and consciously learned, and intensive instruction is “essential” (Ehri, 2004).
Basic Phonics
Basic phonics includes straight-forward
rules, the ones that work well and that students can remember. According to
basic phonics, we learn to read by actually reading and understanding what is
on the page. In fact most of our
knowledge of phonics is the result of reading; the more complex rules of
phonics are subconsciously acquired through reading (Smith, 2003).
A conscious knowledge of some basic rules can
help children learn to read by making texts more comprehensible. Smith (2003)
demonstrates how this can happen: In the sentence, “The man was riding on the
h____.”, the child is unable to read the final word. Given the context and
knowledge of the sound ‘h’ makes, the child can make an intelligent guess as to
what the final word is. This may not work every time (some readers might think
the missing word was “Harley”), but the knowledge of phonics can restrict the
possibilities of what the unknown words may be.
Zero Phonics
This view claims that direct teaching of phonics is not necessary or
even helpful. I am not aware of anyone
who holds the opinion that no phonics should ever be taught.
Complexity of Phonics
An argument against intensive systematic
phonics is that many rules are very complex and do not work very well. As Smith (2003) notes, a considerable number of phonics rules are
“unreliable…there are too many alternatives and exceptions…300 ways in which
letters and sounds can be related” (p. 41).
His most famous example is the fact that each of these uses of “ho” has
a different pronunciation: hot, hoot, hook, hour, honest, house, hope, honey,
and hoist. Smith notes that even if a reader knows the rules, the words cannot
be read accurately from left to right, letter by letter. The reader needs to
look ahead. Smith also notes that different phonics programs teach
different rules, a stunning counterargument to the claim that teaching complex
rules is necessary.
The Limited Impact: The Garan Effect
The National Reading Panel (NICHD, 2000)
concluded that experimental research supports intensive systematic
phonics. Garan (2001, 2002), in an
examination of this report, noted that the impact of intensive phonics is
strong on tests in which children read lists of words in isolation, but it is
miniscule on tests in which children have to understand what they read. Thus,
intensive phonics instruction only helps children to develop the ability to
pronounce words in isolation. Garan's results agree with the results of many
other studies that show that intensive phonics instruction has a positive
impact on tests of decoding but not on tests of comprehension (Krashen, 2009).
Reading experience results in both reading
ability and the ability to do well on tests of "decoding". Children
who have been given the opportunity to do a great deal of interesting,
comprehensible reading and have been given less decoding instruction, perform as well as or better than children in
decoding-emphasis classes on decoding tests. Moreover, they typically score
higher on tests that assess what really counts in reading—comprehension
(Morrow, O'Conner and Smith, 1990; Eldridge, 1991; Klesius, Griffith, and
Zielonka, 1991).
Which Rules?
If the basic phonics position is correct, what
are the rules that are teachable and useful? Experienced professionals agree
that the rules for pronouncing most initial consonants and a few other rules
can be learned and applied to the text by small children, but some rules will
be impossible for six year olds (and most adults). An example of one such rule
recommended by Johnson (2001) is: “the a-e combination is pronounced with the
long vowel and the final e silent (except when the final syllable is
unaccented, then the vowel is pronounced with a short -i sound, as in ‘palace,’
or the combination is ‘are,’ with words such as ‘have’ and ‘dance’ as
exceptions”).
The Great Misunderstanding
There is a strong support among the public
and the media for “phonics” instruction. What is not clear however is whether
the support is for intensive systematic phonics, or basic phonics. Whole language advocates are regularly
accused of supporting the zero phonics position, but they actually support
basic phonics, maintaining that basic phonics is one way to help make texts
more comprehensible. Public opinion
might be quite close to the whole language view.
Late Starters
In
contrast, professional literature contains a significant amount of evidence
showing that starting late can also result in successful reading.
Countries That Start Later
Elley (1992) noted that "… countries which begin instruction in
reading at age seven have largely caught up with the 5- and 6-year old starters
in reading ability by age nine” (p. 37). Table 1 presents reading test scores
for 9-year olds across four countries in which reading instruction began at age
7. Clearly, students who were introduced to reading after age 7 had average
reading scores above the norm by age 9.
|
Score
|
Rank
|
Economic Development**
|
Books
in Home*
|
Finland
|
569
|
1
|
5
|
135
|
Sweden
|
539
|
3
|
2
|
174
|
Norway
|
524
|
7
|
3
|
157
|
Iceland
|
518
|
8
|
4
|
118
|
Table 1: Reading Scores at Age
Nine for Countries in which Reading Instruction began at Age Seven. From: Elley
(1994)
*Books: average number of books in the home
**Economic Development: calculated from GNP,
expenditures for education, life expectancy and other variables
Mean reading score for all 32 countries = 500
It is interesting to note that Finland,
Sweden, Norway and Iceland rank among the highest in the world in economic
development. All four reported that their communities have a plentiful supply
of books in homes and school libraries, and that public libraries and
bookstores were also available to students. Elley’s findings suggest that a
late start is not a problem when children have access to reading materials.
McQuillan
(in Krashen and McQuillan, 2007) reported a number of cases of children who
started reading late, but who had no trouble "catching up", including
home-schooled and dyslexic children, but who learned to read very well.
Home-Schooled Children
Mason (1993a) reports that her daughter,
K.M., “could not/did not want to read” at eight and a half. Having tried
earlier to push her to learn math, and finding that the pressure made her “hate
arithmetic,” Mason decided not to intervene when it came to reading. Around
K.M.‘s ninth birthday, “she began to read, and two months later she could read
at the level of her literate friends. Then she extended her reading, and now
(age 15) she reads the way very literate adults do” (p. 28).
Mason (1993b) describes the case of her son,
D.M.. The summer D.M. turned 10, he could only read a word or two. By fall,
according to his mother, D.M. began “to read store signs and notices with a
vengeance…(One night) sometime past
midnight, he read his way through a fat Spiderman annual his older brother Luke
gave him for his birthday” (p. 11). D.M. also began reading the sports page of
the local newspaper. One day, Mason took D.M. to the local science museum,
where he read aloud “long paragraphs of technical writing discussing ‘atmospheric
conditions’ and ‘helium gases in the stratosphere’” (p. 11).
H.K. (Kerman, 1993) was reading at a “bare
Cat in the Hat level” at the age of 10 and a half. Her mother reports:
“During the course of the next year, she did learn the basics about
reading, although I shall never know how, since she refused instruction as much
as always. We continued to read out loud to her, and she rarely read to
herself. My main consolation was that she loved books and didn’t think badly of
herself. At the age of 14, she started to read Scott O’Dell’s books. The first one took her two months to read.
Two months later, she was reading full-length adult fantasy novels … She reads
voraciously now at the age of 16” (p. 27).
These cases have several features in common—little
or no formal instruction was required, the parents put no pressure on the child
to read, and all of the children made rapid progress once they began reading
material they were genuinely interested in of their own volition. Finally, all
the children had the advantage of having access to a lot of reading material.
Recovered Dyslexics
Another set of cases of readers who started
late but caught up through voluntary reading comes from Fink (1995/6). Fink
studied 12 people who were considered dyslexic when they were young, who all
became “skilled readers”. Out of the 12 people, 9 published creative scholarly
works and one was a Nobel laureate. Eleven out of these people reported that
they finally learned to read between the ages of 10 and 12 (p. 273), and one
did not learn to read until the 12th grade.
According to Fink, these readers had a lot in
common:
“As children, each had a passionate personal interest, a burning desire
to know more about a discipline that required reading. Spurred by this passionate
interest, all read voraciously, seeking and reading everything they could get
their hands on about a single intriguing topic" (pp. 274-275).
Cases of "Late Beginners" in Both Literacy and Creative Work
who Make Profound Contributions
Michael Faraday is a good example of someone
who had little schooling but developed high levels of literacy (academic
literacy) as well as subject matter knowledge. Faraday came from a poor family,
left school before he was 13, and worked for seven years as an apprentice
bookbinder. This meant he had access to a lot of books. His employer “was a
sympathetic and helpful individual who did much to encourage his apprentices’
interests” (Howe, 1999, p. 266). According to Howe, Faraday “read voraciously”
and also attended lectures and classes on his own.
Clearly, Faraday never studied, and never
prepared for examinations. He did a lot of extensive reading when he was a
teenager, including The Arabian Nights
and other novels. Howe speculates that Faraday's interest in science grew
gradually, and became firm when he was around 18 (p. 88). Around the same time,
deeply influenced by the work of Isaac Watts, Faraday began a rigorous
self-study program. Watts emphasized critical and creative reactions to
reading, "...it is the exercise of your own reason and judgment upon all
you read that ... affords your understanding the truest improvement ..."
(as cited in Howe, p. 93)
Working as an assistant to a famous chemist
Humphrey Davy, Faraday took advantage of the facilities available to him and
"plunged into research of his own" (Howe, p. 102) at age 21, and
published his first paper at age 25. Faraday's stunning career after this
consisted of a series of problems he attempted to solve, with great success.
The case of Michael Faraday is consistent
with creativity researcher Simonton's conclusions: "omnivorous reading in
childhood and adolescence correlates positively with ultimate adult
success." (Simonton, 1988, p. 11). We must however, add a commitment to
problem-solving.
Mary Somerville "could scarcely
read" at the age of ten, having grown up in Scotland in the late 1700's, a
time during which girls were often not schooled. A year at a "fashionable" and very
strict girls' school produced no effect, and she returned home and started
pleasure reading, a habit her family disapproved of. By chance, at age 14, she heard about algebra
and geometry, was fascinated by them, and managed to get a copy of Euclid's
Elements of Geometry, which she studied with great interest every night.
"Her mother was appalled and shamed by such aberrant behavior, and
the servants were instructed to confiscate Mary's supply of candles so that she
could not study at night. However, by this time Mary had gone through the first
six books of Euclid…" (Osen, 1974, p. 56).
After years of independent study of math and
some astronomy, Mary was able to dedicate herself to serious scientific work at
the age of 27. She won an award for an
original contribution to a problem published in a mathematics journal, and the
editor became her mentor. The result was a stunning career in mathematics,
astronomy and other areas. Mary Somerville remained productive until she was
89 years old.
Conclusions
The cases and research presented here are
consistent with the following generalizations:
1. Early direct instruction is not effective.
2. Comprehensible, compelling reading works
at all ages.
Early
direct teaching of skills is based on the premise that in learning to read,
skills such as phonemic awareness and phonics must come first; they are
prerequisites to learning to read. There is, however, an impressive amount of
evidence that shows that such "skills" are the result of reading, not
the cause. This is supported by a large number of studies that indicate that
self-selected reading results in greater development of many aspects of
literacy (Krashen, 2004). It is also supported by cases presented earlier in
this paper describing that "late readers" who learned to read by
self-selected reading, and who became quite literate, even though they ignored
formal instruction or had little of it.
3. We can add as a corollary: Compelling
problem-solving produces cognitive development at all ages, as evidenced by the
cases of Michael Faraday and Mary Somerville, who never "learned their
basics," and never studied for tests.
The True Basics
One point that was common to all the
successful late starters described in this paper was that they all had an
environment that provided the essentials—a good diet and adequate health care.
All those who learned to read late, including Fink's recovered dyslexics, had
access to books. Faraday had the good fortune to work for a helpful bookbinder,
and Mary Somerville had access to lighter reading and was eventually was able
to get the texts she was interested in.
In conclusion, those who are interested in
giving children an early start would be better off focusing on supplying the
essentials. In the US, there appears to be more concern around early direct
teaching rather than with providing the essentials—24% of children in the US
live in poverty, which means inadequate diet, inadequate health care, and less
access to books. American educational
policy, however, is currently focusing on early direct teaching enforced
through intensive testing, starting in preschool.
References
Campbell, R. & Butterworth, B. (1985).
Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia in a highly literate subject: A
developmental case with associated deficits and phonemic processing and
awareness. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 37A,
435-475.
Ehri, L. (2004). Teaching phonemic awareness and phonics.
In P. McCardle and V. Chhabra
(Eds.). The voice of evidence.. Baltimore, NY: Paul Brookes. (pp. 153-186)
Eldridge, L. (1991). An experiment with a modified whole language
approach in first-grade classrooms. Reading
Research and Instruction, 30(3),
21-38.
Elley, W.
(1992). How in the World do Students Read? Hamburg: International
Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.
Elley, W.
(1994). Achievement and Instruction in Thirty-Two School Systems: The IEA
Study of Reading Literacy. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Fink, R., (1995/6). Successful
dyslexics: A constructivist study of passionate interest reading.
Journal of
Adolescent and Adult Literacy 39, 268-280.
Garan, E. (2001). Beyond the smoke and mirrors: A
critique of the National Reading Panel report on phonics. Phi Delta Kappan, 82(7), 500-506.
Garen, E. (2002). Resisting reading mandates. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Howe, M. (1999). Genius explained. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Johnson, F. (2001). The utility of phonics
generalizations: Let’s take another look at Clymer’s conclusions. The Reading Teacher, 55, 132-143.
Kerman, K. 1993. A mother learns to understand her child. Growing without Schooling, 92: 27.
Klesius, J., Griffith, P., and Zielonkia, P.
(1991). A whole language and traditional instruction comparison: Overall
effectiveness and development of the alphabetic principle. Reading Research and Instruction, 30, 47-61.
Kozminsky, L. and Kozminsky, E. (1995). The effects of
early phonological awareness training on reading success. Learning and Instruction, 5,187-201.
Krashen, S. (2001a). Does “pure” phonemic awareness training affect
reading comprehension? Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 93, 356-358.
Krashen, S. (2001b). Low PA can
read OK. Practically Primary, 6(3),
17-20.
Krashen, S. (2003). The unbearable coolness
of phonemic awareness. Language Magazine,
2(8), 13-18.
Krashen, S. (2004). The Power of Reading.
Portsmouth: Heinemann and Westport: Libraries Unliminted.
Krashen, S. (2009). Does intensive decoding
instruction contribute to reading comprehension? Knowledge Quest, 37(4),
72-74.
Krashen, S. and
Hastings, A. (2011). Is phonemic awareness training necessary in second
language literacy development? Is it Even Useful? International Journal of Foreign Language Teaching, 7(1).
Krashen, S. and McQuillan, J. (2007). Late
intervention. Educational Leadership, 65(2),
68-73.
Mason, J. (1993a). Without a curriculum. Growing without Schooling, 94, 28.
Mason, J. (1993b). Reading at 10. Growing without Schooling, 91, 11.
Morrow, L., O'Conner E. and Smith, J. (1990).
Effects of a story reading program and literacy development of at-risk
kindergarten children. Journal of Reading
Behavior, 36, 250-275.
National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development (NICHD). (2000). Report of
the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read. NIH Publication Number 00-4654. Washington, DC: NIH.
Osen, M. (1974). Women in mathematics. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Simonton, D. K.
(1988). Scientific Genius: A Psychology
of Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Smith, F.
(2003). Understanding reading. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.